Threatened by an egg predator?Nest guarding is an adaptive strategy which can improve the survival of offspring. However, there is a trade-off, as costs in terms of time, investment of additional nutritional resources, greater risk of predation and loss of additional opportunities to reproduce are incurred by guarding. In environments where parental care can greatly improve offspring survival (eg. predation on eggs and offspring is high), parental care can be a highly successful strategy.
Parental care is a common strategy in freshwater fishes. The extent of parental care can vary greatly, from rudimentary nest building, through to egg guarding by one or both parents, mouth brooding of eggs and larvae, and extended guarding of larvae, and a suite of variations in between. Indeed, ‘male pregnancy’ is an extreme form of male parental care. Purple spotted gudgeons (Mogurnda adspersa) exhibit parental care, whereby the male gudgeon guards the eggs (and in some cases, the larvae), which are spawned in a tight clutch. The male will sit on the eggs and fan them until they hatch, and often stay with the larvae until they disperse. This guarding behaviour is important: a suite of predators (both invertebrate and vertebrate) exist, that would readily eat the clutch of eggs. For the male to be an effective guarder, he must first recognise a potential predator of his brood, and launch a successful defence. What about in the event of a novel predator – one which he has no prior (either personal or evolutionary) experience with? We aimed to test this, using the highly invasive oriental weatherloach. |
The bad guy |
Oriental weatherloach (Misgurnus anguillicaudatus) are an invasive freshwater fish present and spreading rapidly in the Murray Darling basin. Impacts attributable to this species include predation of eggs of native fish, but limited evidence exists. We performed a pilot study using naive purple spotted gudgeon males guarding their eggs, and naive oriental weatherloach to determine whether or not male purple spotted gudgeon could adequately guard their offspring against a novel egg predator. |
Methods:
This study involved 7 successfully breeding male purple spotted gudgeons, housed in their own 2 foot aquaria.
Firstly, a clutch of eggs guarded by each male (the female was removed from the tank after spawning) was measured to determine the size of the egg clutch. This was done on the 2nd day after spawning (every time, to control for developmental effects). The clutch was briefly removed from the aquaria, photographed beside a ruler, and returned to the aquaria. 24 hr later, the clutch was removed and photographed again. This was done to determine the natural rate of clutch decay over a 24 hr period. This was the control, as no predators were present in the aquaria at this time.
When the next clutch of eggs was produced (1–2 weeks later), on the second day post-spawning (again, the female was removed immediately after spawning), the egg clutch was removed, photographed and returned. We also measured 2 behavioural traits of the male at the same time. Firstly, we captured the male with a dip net and removed him from the tank for 10 seconds (held in open air) before returning him to the tank. We then recorded how many seconds he took to return to guarding his eggs. We treated this as an indication of his speed to return to his eggs. Secondly, we recorded over a 15 minute period, how much time he would spend guarding his eggs (his attentiveness).
Following these measurements of the egg clutch size, and the males behavioural traits, we introduced an oriental weatherloach to the tank. Oriental weatherloach were introduced 2 hours before dark (lights out), and trials were completed the following morning. This allowed the oriental weatherloach a whole night to attempt to predate the eggs, and the male gudgeon to successfully guard against predation. We set up a video camera in front of each tank to record the 2 hours of interactions before lights out.
The following morning, we removed the oriental weatherloach, removed the egg clutch and photographed it, and returned it to the aquaria.
This first trial with an oriental weatherloach was conducted with a naive weatherloach. As such, both participants were completely naive.
The trial was repeated again (1–2 weeks later, with a third clutch of eggs). This time, both the male gudgeon and the weatherloach had previously experienced each other (although oriental weatherloach were randomly assigned to males, so it is unknown if the exact same individuals were re-acquainted).
Statistical analysis:
The response variable in this experiment is the proportion of eggs lost from the clutch between the first and second measurement (eg. day 2 and day 3 post-spawning). The treatment effect has three levels (control, naive, and experienced). Additional covariates were not included due to the low number of fish used in each trial, despite there being interesting levels of variation between males. Because each male was used three times, fish ID was included as a random factor (random intercept).
The error distribution was modelled as a betabinomial distribution, due to high overdispersion. The betabinomial distribution includes an additional parameter that estimates the overdispersion. This is somewhat analogous to including an observation-level random intercept to the binomial distribution. Due to the small number of males (n = 7), high overdispersion of variance (yet adequately captured using a betabinomial distribution), and not tracking each weatherloach (which should be included in a one-way cross-nested mixed effects model), I have decided to present the results here as a ‘pilot study’. Analyses were conducted in glmmADMB (version 0.8.0).
This study involved 7 successfully breeding male purple spotted gudgeons, housed in their own 2 foot aquaria.
Firstly, a clutch of eggs guarded by each male (the female was removed from the tank after spawning) was measured to determine the size of the egg clutch. This was done on the 2nd day after spawning (every time, to control for developmental effects). The clutch was briefly removed from the aquaria, photographed beside a ruler, and returned to the aquaria. 24 hr later, the clutch was removed and photographed again. This was done to determine the natural rate of clutch decay over a 24 hr period. This was the control, as no predators were present in the aquaria at this time.
When the next clutch of eggs was produced (1–2 weeks later), on the second day post-spawning (again, the female was removed immediately after spawning), the egg clutch was removed, photographed and returned. We also measured 2 behavioural traits of the male at the same time. Firstly, we captured the male with a dip net and removed him from the tank for 10 seconds (held in open air) before returning him to the tank. We then recorded how many seconds he took to return to guarding his eggs. We treated this as an indication of his speed to return to his eggs. Secondly, we recorded over a 15 minute period, how much time he would spend guarding his eggs (his attentiveness).
Following these measurements of the egg clutch size, and the males behavioural traits, we introduced an oriental weatherloach to the tank. Oriental weatherloach were introduced 2 hours before dark (lights out), and trials were completed the following morning. This allowed the oriental weatherloach a whole night to attempt to predate the eggs, and the male gudgeon to successfully guard against predation. We set up a video camera in front of each tank to record the 2 hours of interactions before lights out.
The following morning, we removed the oriental weatherloach, removed the egg clutch and photographed it, and returned it to the aquaria.
This first trial with an oriental weatherloach was conducted with a naive weatherloach. As such, both participants were completely naive.
The trial was repeated again (1–2 weeks later, with a third clutch of eggs). This time, both the male gudgeon and the weatherloach had previously experienced each other (although oriental weatherloach were randomly assigned to males, so it is unknown if the exact same individuals were re-acquainted).
Statistical analysis:
The response variable in this experiment is the proportion of eggs lost from the clutch between the first and second measurement (eg. day 2 and day 3 post-spawning). The treatment effect has three levels (control, naive, and experienced). Additional covariates were not included due to the low number of fish used in each trial, despite there being interesting levels of variation between males. Because each male was used three times, fish ID was included as a random factor (random intercept).
The error distribution was modelled as a betabinomial distribution, due to high overdispersion. The betabinomial distribution includes an additional parameter that estimates the overdispersion. This is somewhat analogous to including an observation-level random intercept to the binomial distribution. Due to the small number of males (n = 7), high overdispersion of variance (yet adequately captured using a betabinomial distribution), and not tracking each weatherloach (which should be included in a one-way cross-nested mixed effects model), I have decided to present the results here as a ‘pilot study’. Analyses were conducted in glmmADMB (version 0.8.0).
Results:
There was considerable variation in the change of size of egg clutches over the 24 hour duration of the experiment. During the control trial where no predator was present, egg clutch area decreased by an average of 75mm2. Conversely, during the first trial with an oriental weatherloach present (naive), egg clutch area decreased by an average of 317mm2. In the second trial with oriental weatherloach, egg clutch area decreased by 263mm2.
Generalized mixed modelling revealed that there was a significant increase in the area of egg mass consumed in the first trial (naive) compared to the control. There was no significant difference between the second trial (experienced) and the control.
There was considerable variation in the change of size of egg clutches over the 24 hour duration of the experiment. During the control trial where no predator was present, egg clutch area decreased by an average of 75mm2. Conversely, during the first trial with an oriental weatherloach present (naive), egg clutch area decreased by an average of 317mm2. In the second trial with oriental weatherloach, egg clutch area decreased by 263mm2.
Generalized mixed modelling revealed that there was a significant increase in the area of egg mass consumed in the first trial (naive) compared to the control. There was no significant difference between the second trial (experienced) and the control.
Discussion:
These results are very preliminary. Limited conclusions can be drawn from this study, however, it does indicate that oriental weatherloach may pose a threat to native fish eggs, even in the presence of a guarding adult. The role that repeated experiences between both the predator and the guarder remains to be seen. Both learning and co-evolution could play a big role in determining the outcomes of these interactions, and is a fascinating area of future research.
Observations made during this study revealed that male purple spotted gudgeons vary greatly in their aggression towards oriental weatherloach. Some were truly ferocious, while others paid no notice. Likewise, some weatherloach appeared to be more predatory than others. Observations were made of oriental weatherloach consuming eggs, both when a male gudgeon was present, and in a separate trial when the male was removed.
Aquaria trials are a useful way to obtain some preliminary information, however the restricted size of the tanks and limited opportunities for male gudgeons to choose an optimal nesting site may play a bit role in the risk of egg clutches to predation. Exploring this phenomenon in a more natural setting is recommended for future studies.
Future studies should look to incorporate aspects of both male gudgeon and oriental weatherloach behaviour to determine where individual-level behavioural traits may play a role in determining the outcomes of these interactions. Multi-generational studies could look to determine if co-evolutionary responses occur in one or both species.
Acknowledgements:
Data for this study was collected as part of an undergraduate research project by E. Sima. Statistical analyses undertaken by D. Starrs. Funding for this project was generously awarded to D. Starrs by the Linnean Society of New South Wales.
These results are very preliminary. Limited conclusions can be drawn from this study, however, it does indicate that oriental weatherloach may pose a threat to native fish eggs, even in the presence of a guarding adult. The role that repeated experiences between both the predator and the guarder remains to be seen. Both learning and co-evolution could play a big role in determining the outcomes of these interactions, and is a fascinating area of future research.
Observations made during this study revealed that male purple spotted gudgeons vary greatly in their aggression towards oriental weatherloach. Some were truly ferocious, while others paid no notice. Likewise, some weatherloach appeared to be more predatory than others. Observations were made of oriental weatherloach consuming eggs, both when a male gudgeon was present, and in a separate trial when the male was removed.
Aquaria trials are a useful way to obtain some preliminary information, however the restricted size of the tanks and limited opportunities for male gudgeons to choose an optimal nesting site may play a bit role in the risk of egg clutches to predation. Exploring this phenomenon in a more natural setting is recommended for future studies.
Future studies should look to incorporate aspects of both male gudgeon and oriental weatherloach behaviour to determine where individual-level behavioural traits may play a role in determining the outcomes of these interactions. Multi-generational studies could look to determine if co-evolutionary responses occur in one or both species.
Acknowledgements:
Data for this study was collected as part of an undergraduate research project by E. Sima. Statistical analyses undertaken by D. Starrs. Funding for this project was generously awarded to D. Starrs by the Linnean Society of New South Wales.